| Peer-Reviewed

Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances

Received: 14 November 2021    Accepted: 2 December 2021    Published: 24 December 2021
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

Flight response or initiation distances are important for wildlife management because these metrics can quantify changes in human-wildlife relationships such as tolerance and habituation, provide insight into the impacts of predator-prey interactions, and help mitigate human-wildlife conflicts by informing the prescription of minimum distances humans should maintain from wild animals. Despite the importance of flight response measurements, and the ubiquity of their measurement in active wildlife management programs, the relative importance of various environmental and behavioural factors influencing flight response in wild animals remains poorly understood. In this study I used results from 809 flight initiation distance trials on wild adult female elk, including marked individuals for whom I had previously quantified their personalities. These elk were parts of both highly and less habituated herds in the protected area of Banff National Park. Using linear models I identified "personality" and a correlate of wolf predation "risk" as the most important predictors of flight initiation distance. Boldness of personality type predicted lower flight distances, while recent exposure to predation risk increased flight distances, independent of personality. I used Monte Carlo simulations to demonstrate that the accuracy of mean flight response measures could be improved significantly with field methods controlling for personality and group-level risk variations, and that those controlling measures could be obtained using two easily observable correlates: position in herd (bold animals found on the perimeter) and herd clustering (herd structure tighter when currently or recently exposed to risk). I showed that fewer FID trials were necessary to obtain accurate means when these methods applied. Lastly, I showed how few FID trials were needed to obtain an accurate mean for any individual, due to the behavioural consistency of personality. These results showed that personality is the single most important factor influencing variation in ungulate flight initiation distance, followed by exposure to risk, and that both of these factors should be accounted for when collecting flight initiation data.

Published in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (Volume 6, Issue 4)
DOI 10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15
Page(s) 125-135
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Elk, Flight Response, Habituation, Personality, Predation risk, Wildlife Management

References
[1] Hediger H. 1934. Zur Biologie und Psychologie der Flucht bei Tieren. Biologisches Zentralblatt 54: 21-40.
[2] Stankowich T. 2008. Ungulate flight responses to human disturbance: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Conservation 141: 2159-2173.
[3] Moller AP. 2014. Life history, predation and flight initiation distance in a migratory bird. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 27: 1105-1113.
[4] Moller A. 2015. Birds. In W. Cooper, Jr & D. Blumstein (Eds.), Escaping From Predators: An Integrative View of Escape Decisions (pp. 88-112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781107447189.005.
[5] Anchieta J, Nunes CC, Costa Y, Blumstein DT, Leduc A, Dorea AC, Benevides LJ, Sampaio CLS and Barros F. 2018. Global trends on reef fishes’ ecology of fear: Flight initiation distance for conservation. Marine Environmental Research 136: 153-157.
[6] Cooper W. 2015. Reptiles. In W. Cooper, Jr & D. Blumstein (Eds.), Escaping From Predators: An Integrative View of Escape Decisions (pp. 113-151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781107447189.006.
[7] Stankowich T, Blumstein DT. 2005. Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 272: 2627-2634.
[8] Blumstein, D. T. (2000). Understanding antipredator behavior for conservation. The Open Country, 1 (2), 37–44.
[9] Blumstein, D. T. (2003). Flight initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. Journal of Wildlife Management, 67, 852-857.
[10] Knight J. 2009. Making wildlife viewable: habituation and attraction. Journal of Human-Animal Studies 17: 167-184.
[11] Galbreath DM, Ichinose T, Furutani T, Yan W, Higuchi H. 2014. Urbanization and its implications for avian aggression: a case study of urban black kites (Milvus migrans) along Sagami Bay in Japan. Landscape Ecology 29: 169–178.
[12] Conover M. 2002. Resolving human-wildlife conflicts: the science of wildlife damage management. Lewis Publishers.
[13] MacArthur RA, Geist V, Johnston RH. 1982. Cardiac and behavioral responses of mountain sheep to human disturbance. Journal of Wildlife Management 46: 351–358.
[14] Ditmer MA, Vincent JB, Werden LK, Tanner JC, Laske TG, Iaizzo PA, Garshelis DL, Fieberg JR. 2015. Bears show a physiological but limited behavioral response to unmanned aerial vehicles. Current Biology 25: 2278-2283.
[15] Found, R. 2019. Personality influences habituation behaviour in ungulates. Journal of Ethology 37: 47-58.
[16] Rodgers Jr. JA, Smith HT. 1997. Buffer zone disturbances to protect foraging and loafing waterbirds from human disturbance in Florida. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25: 139–145.
[17] Parks Canada. https://www.pc.gc.ca/en/pn-np/ab/banff/visit/avance-ahead/regs/espace-space. Accessed December 10, 2018.
[18] Stankowich T, Coss RG. 2006. Effects of risk assessment, predator behavior, and habitat on escape behavior in Columbian black-tailed deer. Behavioral Ecology, doi: 10.1093/beheco/arl086.
[19] Møller AP. 2008. Flight distance of urban birds, predation and selection for urban life. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 63: 63–75.
[20] Geist V, Stemp RE, Johnston RH. 1985. Heart-rate telemetry of bighorn sheep as a means to investigate disturbances. In: Bayfield, N. G., Barrow, G. C. (Eds.), The Ecological Impacts of Outdoor Recreation on Mountain Areas in Europe and North America, Recreational Ecology Research Group Report, no. 9, Wye College, Wye, pp. 92–99.
[21] Lind J, Cresswell W. 2005. Determining the fitness consequences of antipredation behavior. Behavioral Ecology, doi: 10.1093/beheco/ari075.
[22] Found R, St. Clair CC. 2016. Behavioural syndromes predict loss of migration in wild elk. Animal Behaviour 115: 35-46.
[23] Birke L, Hockenhull J, Creighton E, Pinno L, Mee J, Mills D. 2011. Horses' responses to variation in human approach. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 134: 56-63.
[24] Delaney DK, Gurbb TG, Seibr P, Pater LL, Reiser MH. 1999. Effects of helicopter noise on Mexican spotted owls. Journal of Wildlife Management 63: 60–76.
[25] Bauwens D, Thoen C. 1981. Escape tactics and vulnerability to predation associated with reproduction in the lizard Lacerta vivipara. Journal of Animal Ecology 50: 733–743.
[26] Recarte JM, Vincent JP, Hewison AJM. 1998. Flight response of park fallow deer to the human observer. Behavioural Processes 44: 65-72.
[27] Burger J, Gochfeld M. 1990. Risk discrimination of direct versus tangential approach by basking black iguanas (Ctenosaura similis): Variation as a function of human exposure. Journal of Comparative Psychology 104: 388-394.
[28] Blumstein, D. T., Anthony, L. L., Harcourt, R. & Ross, G. (2002). Testing a key assumption of wildlife buffer zones: is flight initiation distance a species-specific trait? Biological Conservation, 110, 97-100.
[29] Møller AP. 2010. Interspecific variation in fear responses predicts urbanization in birds. Behavavioral Ecology 21:365–371.
[30] Díaz M, Møller AP, Flensted-Jensen E, Grim T, Ibáñez-Álamo JD, Jokimäki J, Markó G, Tryjanowski P. 2013. The geography of fear: a latitudinal gradient in anti-predator escape distances of birds across Europe. PLoS One. 8:e64634.
[31] Kloppers, E. L., St. Clair, C. C. & Hurd, T. E. 2005. Predator-resembling aversive conditioning for managing habituated wildlife. Ecology and Society 10: 31.
[32] Petelle, M. B., McCoy, D. E., Alejandro, V., Martin, J. G. A. & Blumstein, D. T. Development of boldness and docility in yellow-bellied marmots. Animal Behaviour 86: 1147-1154.
[33] Environment Canada. 2021. Historical weather for Banff CS, Alberta. https://climate.weather.gc.ca/climate_data/daily_data_e.html?StationID=27378. Site accessed January 3, 2021.
[34] Paquet, P. C., Wierzchowski, J. & Callaghan, C. 1996. Effects of human activity on gray wolves in the Bow River Valley, Banff National Park, Alberta. Chapter 7: 74-120.
[35] Goldberg, J. F., Hebblewhite, M. & Bardsley, J. 2014. Consequences of a Refuge for the Predator-Prey Dynamics of a Wolf-Elk System in Banff National Park, Alberta, Canada. PLoS One 9.
[36] Ham, S. 2010. Wildlife corridors around developed areas in Banff National Park. Progress report for Parks Canada Warden Service. Winter 2009/2010.
[37] Found R, St. Clair CC. 2017. Ambidextrous ungulates have more flexible behaviour, bolder personalities and migrate less. Royal Society Open Science 4: 160958. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.160958.
[38] Found R. 2015. Ecological implications of personality in elk. PhD Thesis. University of Alberta. Edmonton, Alberta, Canada.
[39] Creel S, Winnie Jr. JA, Christianson D, Liley S. 2008. Time and space in general models of antipredator response: tests with wolves and elk. Animal Behaviour 76: 1139-1146.
[40] Liley S, Creel S. 2007. What best explains vigilance in elk: characteristics of prey, predators, or the environment? Behavioral Ecology 19: 245-254.
[41] Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using Multivariate Statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
[42] Hosmer, D. W. & Lemeshow, S. 2000. Applied logistic regression. Wiley, New York, USA.
[43] Guay, P-J, van Dongen, W. F. D., Robinson, R. W., Blumstein, D. T. & Weston, M. A. 2016. AvianBuffer: An interactive tool for characterizing and managing wildlife fear responses. Ambio 45: 841-851.
[44] Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. 2007. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82: 291-318.
[45] Sih, A., Bell, A., & Johnson, J. C. 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19: 372-378.
[46] Weaver JL. 1994. Ecology of wolf predation amidst high ungulate diversity in Jasper National Parks, Alberta. Thesis. University of Montana, Missoula, Montana, USA.
[47] Moran NP, Sanchez-Tojar A., Schielzeth H, Reinhold K. 2020. Poor nutritional condition promotes high-risk behaviours: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Reviews 96: 269-288. DOI: 10.1111/brv.12655.
[48] Gravolin, I., Key, M. & Lill, A. 2014. Boldness of urban Australian magpies and local traffic volume. Avian Biology Research 7. DOI: 10.3184/175815514X14151981691872.
[49] Rupia E. J., Binnin, S. A., Roche, D. G. & Lu, W. 2016. Fight-flight or freeze-hide? Personality and metabolic phenotype mediate physiological defense responses in flatfish. Journal of Animal Ecology 85: 927-937.
[50] Bejder L, Samuels A, Whitehead H, Finn H, Allen S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitization and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Marine Ecology Progress Series 395: 177-185.
[51] Domjan M. 2010. The Principles of Learning and Behavior. Sixth Edition. Thomson Wadsworth, Belmont, CA, USA.
[52] Found, R., & St. Clair, C. C. 2018. Personality Influences Wildlife Responses to Aversive Conditioning. Journal of Wildlife Management 82: 747-755.
[53] Wam HK, Eldegard K, Hjeljord O. 2014. Minor habituation to repeated experimental approaches in Scandinavian wolves. European Journal of Wildlife Research 60: 839-842.
[54] Ferrari MCO, Elvidge CK, Jackson CD, Chivers DP, Brown GE. 2010. The responses of prey fish to temporal variation in predation risk: sensory habituation or risk assessment? Behavioural Ecology: doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq023.
[55] Samuni L, Mundry R, Terkel, J, Zuberbuhler K, Hobaiter C. 2014. Socially learned habituation to human observers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 17: 997-1005.
[56] Malo JE, Acebes P, Traba J. 2011. Measuring ungulate tolerance to human with flight distance: a reliable visitor management tool? Biological Conservation 20: 3477-3488.
[57] Mech LD, Peterson RO. 2003. Wolf-prey relations. In: Mech, L. D, Boitani, L. (eds), Wolves: behaviour, ecology and conservation. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, USA, pp. 131-157.
[58] Carter AJ, Feeney WE, Marshall HH, Cowlishaw G, Heinsohn R. 2013. Animal personality: what are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biological Reviews 88: 465-475.
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Rob Found. (2021). Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances. Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, 6(4), 125-135. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Rob Found. Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances. Ecol. Evol. Biol. 2021, 6(4), 125-135. doi: 10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Rob Found. Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances. Ecol Evol Biol. 2021;6(4):125-135. doi: 10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15,
      author = {Rob Found},
      title = {Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances},
      journal = {Ecology and Evolutionary Biology},
      volume = {6},
      number = {4},
      pages = {125-135},
      doi = {10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.eeb.20210604.15},
      abstract = {Flight response or initiation distances are important for wildlife management because these metrics can quantify changes in human-wildlife relationships such as tolerance and habituation, provide insight into the impacts of predator-prey interactions, and help mitigate human-wildlife conflicts by informing the prescription of minimum distances humans should maintain from wild animals. Despite the importance of flight response measurements, and the ubiquity of their measurement in active wildlife management programs, the relative importance of various environmental and behavioural factors influencing flight response in wild animals remains poorly understood. In this study I used results from 809 flight initiation distance trials on wild adult female elk, including marked individuals for whom I had previously quantified their personalities. These elk were parts of both highly and less habituated herds in the protected area of Banff National Park. Using linear models I identified "personality" and a correlate of wolf predation "risk" as the most important predictors of flight initiation distance. Boldness of personality type predicted lower flight distances, while recent exposure to predation risk increased flight distances, independent of personality. I used Monte Carlo simulations to demonstrate that the accuracy of mean flight response measures could be improved significantly with field methods controlling for personality and group-level risk variations, and that those controlling measures could be obtained using two easily observable correlates: position in herd (bold animals found on the perimeter) and herd clustering (herd structure tighter when currently or recently exposed to risk). I showed that fewer FID trials were necessary to obtain accurate means when these methods applied. Lastly, I showed how few FID trials were needed to obtain an accurate mean for any individual, due to the behavioural consistency of personality. These results showed that personality is the single most important factor influencing variation in ungulate flight initiation distance, followed by exposure to risk, and that both of these factors should be accounted for when collecting flight initiation data.},
     year = {2021}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Personality and Other Factors Mediating Ungulate Flight Initiation Distances
    AU  - Rob Found
    Y1  - 2021/12/24
    PY  - 2021
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15
    DO  - 10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15
    T2  - Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
    JF  - Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
    JO  - Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
    SP  - 125
    EP  - 135
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2575-3762
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.eeb.20210604.15
    AB  - Flight response or initiation distances are important for wildlife management because these metrics can quantify changes in human-wildlife relationships such as tolerance and habituation, provide insight into the impacts of predator-prey interactions, and help mitigate human-wildlife conflicts by informing the prescription of minimum distances humans should maintain from wild animals. Despite the importance of flight response measurements, and the ubiquity of their measurement in active wildlife management programs, the relative importance of various environmental and behavioural factors influencing flight response in wild animals remains poorly understood. In this study I used results from 809 flight initiation distance trials on wild adult female elk, including marked individuals for whom I had previously quantified their personalities. These elk were parts of both highly and less habituated herds in the protected area of Banff National Park. Using linear models I identified "personality" and a correlate of wolf predation "risk" as the most important predictors of flight initiation distance. Boldness of personality type predicted lower flight distances, while recent exposure to predation risk increased flight distances, independent of personality. I used Monte Carlo simulations to demonstrate that the accuracy of mean flight response measures could be improved significantly with field methods controlling for personality and group-level risk variations, and that those controlling measures could be obtained using two easily observable correlates: position in herd (bold animals found on the perimeter) and herd clustering (herd structure tighter when currently or recently exposed to risk). I showed that fewer FID trials were necessary to obtain accurate means when these methods applied. Lastly, I showed how few FID trials were needed to obtain an accurate mean for any individual, due to the behavioural consistency of personality. These results showed that personality is the single most important factor influencing variation in ungulate flight initiation distance, followed by exposure to risk, and that both of these factors should be accounted for when collecting flight initiation data.
    VL  - 6
    IS  - 4
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada

  • Sections