| Peer-Reviewed

Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts

Received: 25 June 2015     Accepted: 29 June 2015     Published: 10 July 2015
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

Numerous studies have been focused on corrective feedback (CF), specifically recasts. These studies have identified, classified, and explained their acquisitional effects. In terms of acquisitional effects, however, it seems that the studies have not yet covered all areas of grammar which constitutes a very broad range of issues. This study was an attempt to address this problem to some extent by investigating the effects of implicit and explicit recasts on the acquisition of simple perfect tenses (simple present, past, and future perfect tenses). In a within-between pretest-posttest group design, 45 Iranian students of English as a foreign language were assigned to two experimental and one control groups and underwent a treatment which lasted for 15 sessions. The students then were tested on their ability in correct application of simple perfect tenses. Following that, the collected data were subjected to different statistical tests, including MANOVA. The final results affirmed the effectiveness of recasts in improving the acquisition of simple perfect tenses. Of the two types of recasts—explicit vs. implicit—used in the study, explicit recasts proved to be more effective. Among the three tenses studied simple present perfect and simple future perfect tenses seemed to be more significantly affected.

Published in International Journal of Language and Linguistics (Volume 3, Issue 4)
DOI 10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20
Page(s) 258-265
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2015. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Implicit, Explicit, Recast

References
[1] Ammar, A., & Spada, N. (2006). One size fits all? Recasts, prompts, and L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 543–544.
[2] Doughty, C., & Williams, J. (1998). Pedagogical choices in focus on form. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on forming classroom second language (pp. 177–196). New York: Cambridge University Press.
[3] Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS: And sex and drugs and rock 'n' roll (3rded.). London: Sage Publications Ltd.
[4] Harley, B., & Swain, M. (1984). The interlanguage of immersion students and its implication for second language teaching. In A. Davis, C. Criper, &A.Howatt (Eds.), Interlanguage(pp. 291–311). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
[5] Ishida, M. (2004). Effects of recasts on the acquisition of the aspectual form of –Te (ru) by Learners of Japanese as a foreign language. Language Learning,54,311–394.
[6] Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language development: Beyond negative evidence.Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 37–63.
[7] Lightbown, P. M., Halter, R. H., White, J. L., & Horst, M. (2002). Comprehension-based learning: The limits of "do it yourself". Canadian Modern Language Review, 58, 427–464.
[8] Lightbown, P. M., &Spada, N. (1990). Focus-on-form and corrective feedback in communicative language teaching: Effects on second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 429–448.
[9] Lightbown, P. M., &Spada, N. (1994).An innovative for primary ESL in Quebec.TESOL Quarterly, 28,563–579.
[10] Loewen, S., &Philp, J. (2006). Recast in The Adult English L2 Classroom: Characteristics,Explicitness, and effectiveness. The Modern Language Journal,90, 536–556.
[11] Long, M. H. (1991). Focus on form: A design feature in language teaching methodology. In K. D. Bot, C. Kramsch, & R. Ginsberg (Eds.), Foreign language research in cross-cultural perspective (pp. 39–52). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
[12] Long, M. (1996).The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W.R., Ritchie and T.J., Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition. 68–413.Academic Press.
[13] Long, M. H., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on forming classroom second language acquisition (pp. 21-26). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[14] Long, M., Inagaki, S., & Ortega, L. (1998). The role of implicit negative feedback in SLA: Models and recasts in Japanese and Spanish. Modern Language Journal, 82, 357–371.
[15] Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 37–66.
[16] Mackey, A. (2000). Interactional feedback on L2 morpho-syntax: Learners' perceptions and developmental outcome. Paper presented at the annual meeting of AmericanAssociation for Applied Linguistic, Vancouver, B. C., Canada.
[17] Mackey, A., & Philp, J. (1998). Conversational interaction and second language development. Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern Language Journal, 82,338–356.
[18] Moinzadeh, A., Rassaei, E., & youhannaee, M. (2012). Recasts, modified output and L2 development: A case of Persian EFL. English Language and Literature Studies, 2, 100–111.
[19] Pazoki, S., & Seyed Rezaei, F. (2013). The role of recast on left hemisphere dominant vs. right hemisphere dominant Iranian EFL learners. Journal of Advances in English Language Teaching, 1, 47–58.
[20] Rutherford, W., & Sharwood Smith, M. (1985). Consciousness-raising and universal grammar. Applied Linguistics, 6, 274–282.
[21] Schmit, R. (1983). Interaction, accumulation, and the acquisition communicative competence. In n. Wolfson & E. Judd (Eds.), sociolinguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 85-103). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
[22] Schmit, R.(1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 13, 206–226.
[23] Schmit, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognitive and second language Instruction (pp. 1–32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[24] Spada, N. (1997). Formed-focused instruction and second language acquisition: A review of classroom and laboratory research. Language Teaching, 29, 1–15.
[25] White, L. (1987). Against comprehensible input: The input hypothesis and the development of L2 competence. Applied Linguistics, 8, 95–110.
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Sepehr Enjili, Asgar Mahmoudi. (2015). Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts. International Journal of Language and Linguistics, 3(4), 258-265. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Sepehr Enjili; Asgar Mahmoudi. Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts. Int. J. Lang. Linguist. 2015, 3(4), 258-265. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Sepehr Enjili, Asgar Mahmoudi. Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts. Int J Lang Linguist. 2015;3(4):258-265. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20,
      author = {Sepehr Enjili and Asgar Mahmoudi},
      title = {Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts},
      journal = {International Journal of Language and Linguistics},
      volume = {3},
      number = {4},
      pages = {258-265},
      doi = {10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ijll.20150304.20},
      abstract = {Numerous studies have been focused on corrective feedback (CF), specifically recasts. These studies have identified, classified, and explained their acquisitional effects. In terms of acquisitional effects, however, it seems that the studies have not yet covered all areas of grammar which constitutes a very broad range of issues. This study was an attempt to address this problem to some extent by investigating the effects of implicit and explicit recasts on the acquisition of simple perfect tenses (simple present, past, and future perfect tenses). In a within-between pretest-posttest group design, 45 Iranian students of English as a foreign language were assigned to two experimental and one control groups and underwent a treatment which lasted for 15 sessions. The students then were tested on their ability in correct application of simple perfect tenses. Following that, the collected data were subjected to different statistical tests, including MANOVA. The final results affirmed the effectiveness of recasts in improving the acquisition of simple perfect tenses. Of the two types of recasts—explicit vs. implicit—used in the study, explicit recasts proved to be more effective. Among the three tenses studied simple present perfect and simple future perfect tenses seemed to be more significantly affected.},
     year = {2015}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Learning Simple Perfect Tenses Using Implicit and Explicit Recasts
    AU  - Sepehr Enjili
    AU  - Asgar Mahmoudi
    Y1  - 2015/07/10
    PY  - 2015
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20
    DO  - 10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20
    T2  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    JF  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    JO  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    SP  - 258
    EP  - 265
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2330-0221
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20150304.20
    AB  - Numerous studies have been focused on corrective feedback (CF), specifically recasts. These studies have identified, classified, and explained their acquisitional effects. In terms of acquisitional effects, however, it seems that the studies have not yet covered all areas of grammar which constitutes a very broad range of issues. This study was an attempt to address this problem to some extent by investigating the effects of implicit and explicit recasts on the acquisition of simple perfect tenses (simple present, past, and future perfect tenses). In a within-between pretest-posttest group design, 45 Iranian students of English as a foreign language were assigned to two experimental and one control groups and underwent a treatment which lasted for 15 sessions. The students then were tested on their ability in correct application of simple perfect tenses. Following that, the collected data were subjected to different statistical tests, including MANOVA. The final results affirmed the effectiveness of recasts in improving the acquisition of simple perfect tenses. Of the two types of recasts—explicit vs. implicit—used in the study, explicit recasts proved to be more effective. Among the three tenses studied simple present perfect and simple future perfect tenses seemed to be more significantly affected.
    VL  - 3
    IS  - 4
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of English, Ardabil Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Ardabil, Iran

  • Department of English Language, Ardabil Branch, Islamic Azad University, Ardabil, Iran

  • Sections